Including:
More on language developmentLanguage developmentTerms you should know:
The preoperational stage and characteristics thereof
Information processing
In the previous chapter, on infant
language development, you learned about the stages of language development,
theories about the genetic basis of language, and the importance
of the environment. (Click here if you would like
to go back to those pages and review that information.) There are other
common characteristics of children's language development, in addition
to the stages. These include:
The preoperational stage and characteristics thereof:
- Mastery of grammar - by age three, children have significant understanding of the rules of a language (whatever language they happen to speak). For example, you will hear a young child say - "I pet doggy" not "Doggy me pet". They understand that the subject comes first, then the verb, the object. This is quite a complicated understanding, when you really think about it.
- Difficulty in understanding abstract language - young children children do not understand the meaning of such abstract terms as "justice" or "democracy" because it does not relate to their own experience. They also have trouble with metaphors. For example, if Heather's mom comments,
"After all of those piano lessons and Matt still can't play. Well, I guess you can lead a horse to water but you can't make him drink."
Heather is likely to reply:
"Matt doesn't have a horse."
- Overregularization - young children tend to apply the rules of language too much and not make exceptions to the rules when appropriate. For example, a child might say,
"The baby drinked her bottle." or
"I runned when I heared the noise."
The child is applying the rule "add -ed to make a past tense" to every situation, even when it is not appropriate.
- Failure to use the passive tense - this has to do with reversibility (or lack thereof) and overregularization. Young children do not understand sentences such as
"He was bitten by the dog."
They do not understand that the action something was performed on does not always have to go last and that the actor (dog) does not always have to go first. Nor do they realize that you could change this statement around to say "The dog bit him."
In fact, if you made such a statement as ,
"He was bitten by the dog."
a young child is likely to argue,
"No! The dog bited him!" and if you are silly enough to argue that was what you said, the child will correct you and say,
"No, you didn't, you said that the dog bited him!"
(Notice the clever way that I threw examples of overregularization in there, too! (- : )
What are operations? What is he talking about? Maybe Piaget is more easily comprehended in the original French. Unfortunately, I don't read French, do you? Fortunately, you have me, your humble instructor here at your service to (hopefully) make this somewhat clearer than mud.
An example might help...
|
I.E., ONE WHO DOES NOT ENGAGE IN MENTAL OPERATIONS O O O O O O O O O O ME: Now, Maria, look carefully at the row of pennies.
There are ten pennies here, right?
(Notice my clever use of positive reinforcement, in keeping
with what theorist - that's right, Skinner.)
O O O O O O O O O O ME: Now, Maria, are there more pennies than there were
before, less, or just the same?
Maria takes all the pennies.
Why is this preoperational? She got the right answer.
Because Maria had to count the pennies to know that they were the same.
She could not perform logical operations to come to the right answer.
|
The next year, we did the exact same task and the conversation
went like this.
Again, I took a row of ten pennies and evenly spaced them apart. O O O O O O O O O O ME: Now, Maria, look carefully at the row of pennies.
There are ten pennies here, right?
O O O O O O O O O O ME: Now, Maria, are there more pennies than there were
before, less, or just the same?
PAY ATTENTION TO THAT LAST STATEMENT. Although it may not sound so profound, it demonstrates an understanding of the concept of reversibility --- Unlike the year before, she did not need to count the pennies or do anything physical to know that there were the same number of pennies. While a preoperational child could get the correct answer by pushing the pennies back together, a child who has achieved the concrete operational stage can perform this IN HER MIND. That, is, she can know something - in this case, that you can push the pennies back together and have the same appearance as before- without actually doing it. |
Developmental psychologists are in almost universal agreement that Piaget underestimated the cognitive capabilities of young children. As has been pointed out in much of this chapter, children are better than Piaget had thought at understanding causality, other people's points of view and numbers. Your textbook author spends quite a bit of this chapter discussing Wellman's research on theory of the mind, and other studies which seem to document children's ability to understand the world. I am assuming that there is a point to this, rather than just trying to fill up space, and I think the point is that preschool children are quite aware of what is going on around them. SO ... we should:
Click here to go on to the page
on information processing (it is relatively short).